SEMINAR Understanding Dogs and Dognition: A New Foundation for Design Garrison W. Cottrell Department of Dog Science Condominium Community College of Southern California There is a crisis in Dog-Human relations, as has been evidenced by recent attempts to make dogs more "user-friendly" (see Programming the User-Friendly Dog, Cottrell 1985a). A new approach has appeared (Whineandpoop and Flossy, 1986) that claims that previous attempts at Dog-Human Interfaces have floundered on a basic misunderstanding of the Dog. The problem has been that we have approached the Dog as if he was one of us - and he certainly is not. Their perusal of the philosophies of Holedigger and Mateyourauntie has led them to a new understanding: A West Coast Understanding. There is no Objective Reality[1] that we form internal representations of, rather, organisms are structurally coupled[2] to their environment, the so-called "seamless web" theory of cognition. Thus the inside/outside dichotomy that has plagued AI researchers and dogs for years is a false one[3]. This has led them to a whole new way of understanding how dogs should be programmed. In the past we have assumed some internal representation in the dog's head (see Modelling the Intentional Behavior of the Dog, Cottrell 1984b). In this new view, the reason dogs are so dense is not that they have impoverished internal representations, but that they don't have internal representations. Instead, the dog is structurally coupled to the world - he moves about embedded in the ooze of the environment, and naturally, it slows him down. Not only that, but it is the wrong environment - the human one, leading to continual breakdown[4]. Thus our problem is in forming a consensual domain with another species. We have to place ourselves in their domain to hear them - this is termed "listening in the backyard". We feel that there is much to be gained from combining their view with the connectionist approach[5]. The problem is combining the intensional programming of evolution with extensional programming by the owner. Connectionist theories of learning combined with considerations of "listening in the backyard" suggest that if we simply present the dog with many examples of the desired input-output behavior within the backyard, we will get the desired result. ____________________ [1]Actually, Californians have known this for years. [2]Note that this is to be distinguished from the structural coupling that produces new dogs from old ones. [3]Dogs have often followed Mateyourauntie in this, ignoring the inside/outside dichotomy. These considerations may eliminate the basis for the continence-performance distinction (Hutchins, 1986). [4]The field of Dog-Machine Interfaces attempts to deal with such problems as the poor design of the doorknob - a lever would help reduce the inside/outside barrier. Others feel that this research is misdirected; the doorknob is designed that way pre- cisely because it acts as a species filter, keeping dogs out of restaurants and movie theatres. [5]Their work also suggests applying the theory of speech acts to the command interface. Thus, we can classify much more than simple Directives. For example, "You've had it now, Jellybean!" is a commissive - the speaker is committed to a future course of action. The dog will usually respond with an attempt to withdraw from the dialogue, but the speaker rejects his withdrawal. "You're in the doghouse, Bean" is a declarative - the speaker brings about a correspondence between the propositional content of this and reality simply by uttering it.